Frontiers

您所在的位置:网站首页 regulation model Frontiers

Frontiers

#Frontiers | 来源: 网络整理| 查看: 265

HYPOTHESIS AND THEORY article Front. Psychol., 22 May 2018Sec. Developmental Psychology Volume 9 - 2018 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00789 The Face-to-Face Still-Face (FFSF) Paradigm in Clinical Settings: Socio-Emotional Regulation Assessment and Parental Support With Infants With Neurodevelopmental Disabilities Lorenzo Giusti Livio Provenzi* Rosario Montirosso 0-3 Center for the at-Risk Infant, Scientific Institute IRCCS Eugenio Medea, Bosisio Parini, Italy

Background: The Face-to-Face Still-Face (FFSF) paradigm is a well-acknowledged procedure to assess socio-emotional regulation in healthy and at-risk infants. Although it was developed mainly for research purposes, the FFSF paradigm has potential clinical implications for the assessment of socio-emotional regulation of infants with neurodevelopmental disabilities (ND) and to supporting parenting.

Aim: The present paper describes the application of the FFSF paradigm as an evaluation and intervention tool in clinical practice with infants with ND and their parents.

Methods: Theoretical and methodological insights for the use of the FFSF paradigm in the clinical setting are provided. Single-case vignettes from clinical practice further illustrate and provide exemplifications for the use of the FFSF with infants with ND and their parents.

Results: From a clinical point of view, the use of the FFSF paradigm (1) offers a unique observational perspective on socio-emotional regulation in infants with ND and (2) enhances parents’ sensitivity to their infants’ behavior.

Discussion: The FFSF paradigm appears to be a useful tool for clinical assessment of socio-emotional regulation in infants with ND and promote the quality of parenting and early parent-infant interaction.

Introduction

The Face-to-Face Still-Face (FFSF) paradigm (Tronick et al., 1978) is a well-known and validated procedure to assess socio-emotional regulation in infants facing a social stressor. The FFSF paradigm has been widely used with healthy and at-risk infants at different ages and it has contributed to improve our knowledge and conceptualization of early socio-emotional regulation development (Mesman et al., 2009). The suitability of this observational procedure to clinical settings has already been suggested (Miron et al., 2009). However, to the best of our knowledge, the FFSF has not been previously used to assess infants (and children) with a neurodevelopmental disability (ND; e.g., autism spectrum disorders, cerebral palsy, genetic syndromes) in a neurorehabilitation clinical context.

Our perspective is framed by the mutual regulation model (MRM; Gianino and Tronick, 1988). The MRM argues that the parent-infant interaction is organized by a bidirectional exchange of communicative signals that are used by the infant and the caregiver to coordinate the interaction and to cope with the stress of normally occurring interactive ruptures. From this perspective, the quality of the interaction is determined by the ability of each participant to cope with external stressors, regulate his/her emotional states, express communicative messages, and respond to his/her partner’s affective communications and regulatory needs. Caregivers’ behavior is guided by infants’ expressive displays (e.g., gaze, facial expressions, gestures, and vocalizations). In turn, infant behavioral and affective states are affected by the expressive displays of the caregiver. Importantly, the MRM – rather than emphasizing just synchrony – highlights that mother–infant interaction is a process characterized by matching and mismatching and that reparation of mismatches is a key developmental mechanism (Tronick and Beeghly, 2011).

Early caregiver–infant relationship may be strongly affected by the presence of a ND and many challenges arise for parenting in the contexts of infants’ disability. On the one hand, the parents face an augmented emotional burden (Dykens, 2015). On the other hand, infants with ND are less responsive and attentive, make fewer vocal and affective signals, are fussier, and produce less clear social cues (Okimoto et al., 2000). Moreover, infants (and children) with a ND have limited regulatory abilities and, therefore, they need their caregivers’ regulatory scaffolding to maintain emotional regulation and cope with interactive stress (Hauser-Cram and Woodman, 2016). Overall, these factors can disrupt the development of the functional dyadic co-regulatory system – including the reparation of mismatched process – and might further impact on infants’ socio-emotional regulation and parenting behavior. Additionally, it should be highlighted that infants with ND need to be hospitalized with their parents to take part in early rehabilitation programs (i.e., speech therapy, physical therapy, and so on). These programs recognize the importance of supporting parent-infant interaction as part of their daily work, but this focus is not widely implemented in practice (Innocenti et al., 2013). Moreover, having a broad picture of the infant functioning and parent–infant interactive patterns can be useful in the promotion of parenting skills.

From this perspective, we propose that the FFSF paradigm may be effectively used in clinical neurorehabilitation settings. Specifically, the FFSF provides a unique observational procedure for the assessment of infants’ socio-emotional regulation and the quality of early interaction in infants with ND and their parents. Importantly, according to a collaborative consultation approach (Boukydis, 2012), we used the FFSF videotapes to offer caregivers a brief parenting intervention aimed at enhancing parents’ sensitivity and responsiveness to their infant by helping them to observe and reflect upon the infant’s behavior.

Therefore, the general goal of the present paper is to provide a theoretical framework and methodological insights to use the FFSF in clinical settings specialized in the assessment of infants with ND and early parenting intervention. First, we provide a theoretical rationale integrating evidence from the FFSF paradigm research field and literature on socio-emotional regulation and parenting associated with ND. Second, we provide methodological insights for the application of the FFSF paradigm as a valid tool in the clinical setting dealing with ND, both for assessment purposes and parents’ support. Finally, we present clinical examples from our clinical practice highlighting the unique contributions of the FFSF procedure in this context.

The FFSF Paradigm: A Privileged View on Infants’ Socio-Emotional Regulation Development and Parenting Behavior

The ‘70s have been a period of wide-spread growth for the infant research field (e.g., Stern and Stern, 2012; Sander et al., 2014). Within this field of research, the FFSF paradigm was developed by Tronick et al. (1978) to test the hypothesis that infants are active contributors in social interactions and to evaluate how they respond to the violation of interactive and social contingencies in the relationship with their main caregiver. In the FFSF paradigm, caregiver and infant engage in a 2-min-long face-to-face interaction (i.e., Play episode). Subsequently, the caregiver is instructed to stop any communication directed to the infant and to maintain a still face while keeping eye contact with the infant for 2 min (i.e., Still-Face episode). The lack of a contingent caregiver response is stressful for the infant who exhibits the typical still-face effect, which includes heightened negative emotionality as well as a reduction of social engagement and display of avoiding behaviors (Weinberg and Tronick, 1996; Weinberg et al., 1999; Montirosso et al., 2015a; Provenzi et al., 2015a). After the still-face exposure, the caregiver and the infant resume normal face-to-face interaction for another 2 min (i.e., Reunion episode). During the Reunion episode, a carry-over effect is usually observed, which consists in the persistent exhibition of negative emotionality and distress signals (e.g., avoiding behaviors) during the very first moments of the interaction (Weinberg and Tronick, 1996). Usually, during the Reunion, the mother–infant dyad gradually reaches a new homeostatic equilibrium and regains reciprocal positive interaction and playful exchanges (Montirosso et al., 2015a).

The FFSF paradigm has been used to assess developmental trajectories of infants’ emotional regulation (Hsu and Jeng, 2008; Yato et al., 2008), individual differences in behavioral stress response (Weinberg et al., 1999; Montirosso et al., 2015b), socio-cognitive domains such as episodic memory (Montirosso et al., 2013, 2014) as well as physiological reactivity (Muller et al., 2015; Provenzi et al., 2015b; Montirosso et al., 2016a,b; Provenzi et al., 2017). Moreover, the FFSF paradigm has also been used to investigate socio-emotional regulation in developmental risk conditions, such as preterm infants (Segal et al., 1995; Montirosso et al., 2010, 2016b), infants of depressed or anxious mothers (Weinberg et al., 2006; Kaitz et al., 2010; Reck et al., 2013).

Moreover, the interactive nature of the FFSF also allows researchers to assess parenting contribution to infants’ socio-emotional regulation. Different characteristics of parental interactive behavior have been found to be supportive of better socio-emotional regulation in infants: maternal sensitivity associates with more positive emotionality during the Still-Face episode (Braungart-Rieker et al., 2001; Chow et al., 2010); maternal social engagement associates with better regulation and less distress signals in the infant (Lowe et al., 2006; Montirosso et al., 2015b). Recently, one study has documented that infant and maternal behavior during normal face-to-face interactions (e.g., the Play episode) are both significant predictors of the infant’s ability to deal with socio-emotional stress (e.g., the Still-Face episode) at 6 months (Provenzi et al., 2016c).

Socio-Emotional Regulation and Parenting Behavior in Infants With ND

Although they may present very different etiological, genetic and phenotypic characteristics, infants with ND often share a common pattern of difficulties in socio-emotional and behavioral regulation (Hauser-Cram and Woodman, 2016). Socio-emotional dysregulation has been reported in infants and children with cerebral palsy (Odding et al., 2006; Sigurdardottir et al., 2010), children diagnosed with an autism spectrum disorder (Mazefsky and White, 2014; Berkovits et al., 2017) as well as infants with genetic syndromes (e.g., Williams syndrome, Einfeld et al., 2001; Down syndrome, Jahromi et al., 2012). Previous FFSF research further highlighted difficulties in socio-emotional regulation in infants with ND. For example, the response of 3–13-month-old infants with Down syndrome lacked the typical reduction of positive emotionality to the Still-Face episode (Carvajal and Iglesias, 1997). Moreover, preschool children with a diagnosis of autism spectrum disorder reported the typical still-face effect but exhibited immature regulatory behaviors compared to age-matched typically developing children (Ostfeld-Etzion et al., 2015). Importantly, FFSF research also showed that parental behavior contributes to socio-emotional regulation even in infants with ND: in children with an autism spectrum disorder, maternal regulatory support was found to have a buffering effect on infants’ stress reactivity (Ostfeld-Etzion et al., 2015).

Furthermore, it should be noted that parenting assumes a different significance when an infant and child is diagnosed with a ND. Different dimensions of parenting are affected by the presence of a ND in infants, for example the ability to read the infant’s signals, to respond contingently and to provide adequate stimulations and sustain infants’ attention (Landry et al., 2008; Innocenti et al., 2013). Parents may face critical emotional burden (Dykens, 2015) and the acceptance of their own infant’s diagnosis may end up in emotional disturbances, such as high and chronic levels of distress, depression and anxiety (Baird et al., 2000; Papaeliou et al., 2012; Bemister et al., 2015; Cianfaglione et al., 2015; Cohrs and Leslie, 2017). Parents who have unresolved feelings about their infants’ ND also appear to be less able to provide regulatory support (Marvin and Pianta, 1996) and the reduction in maternal sensitivity has been found to be a key predictor of later socio-emotional development of their infants and children (Azad et al., 2013). At the same time, infants with ND may only partially give intelligible signals of their emotional states and needs (Okimoto et al., 2000). The difficulty in parental interpretation and appropriate responsiveness to infants’ cues and communications may further lead to the emergence of directive (Guralnick et al., 2008) or intrusive parenting style (Venuti et al., 2009; Bornstein et al., 2012; Blacher et al., 2013). Globally, these findings show that parent–infant relationship in the context of ND has a heightened risk of incurring in dysfunctional interactive transactions.

Early Parental Interventions in Infants With ND

In light of this evidence, parenting behavior appears to play a key role in supporting infants’ socio-emotional development, even in the presence of ND. Early interventions that engage parents in assessment and rehabilitation phases have better long-lasting effects on developmental trajectories of infants and children with ND (Spittle et al., 2015). Additionally, investing in early interventions is also beneficial from a socio-economic point of view, as it guarantees a major economic return for healthcare systems (Doyle et al., 2009). Previous research that aimed to support parenting in families of infants with ND by using collaborative consultation on videotaped parent–infant interactions (Kim and Mahoney, 2005; Phaneuf and McIntyre, 2007; Lam-Cassettari et al., 2015) reported better outcomes for parental sensitivity and attunement as well as for infants’ behavioral stability and development. In recent years, an increasing number of intervention studies have used the video-feedback method in which infant behavior observation is performed with parents by a trained consultant (Juffer et al., 2012). Different approaches to the video-feedback are described in literature with the common goal of enhancing parental sensitivity and better behavioral strategies (for a review, Fukkink, 2008). The joint observation focuses on the observable interaction between parent and infant and has found to be effective in helping parents to recognize signs of infant’s stress, socio-emotional regulation strategies and parenting behavior also in dyads of infants with ND (Phaneuf and McIntyre, 2007; Poslawsky et al., 2015). The appropriateness of using FFSF paradigm within video-supported collaborative consultation with parents has been already suggested. For example, Papousek (2007) reported that using FFSF with parents can be effective in “recruiting the parent’s intuitive competence and restoring intersubjective emotional relatedness as a basis for the infant’s recovery of communicative growth” (p. 265). However, no previous study has specifically addressed the use of FFSF as an observational procedure for clinical assessment of infant socio-emotional regulation and the quality of early interaction in infants with ND and their parents.

The FFSF Paradigm in Clinical Practice Subjects and Setting

We use the FFSF paradigm with 4–36-month-old infants diagnosed with a range of ND including cerebral palsy, autism spectrum disorders, genetic syndromes, and other mental retardation conditions. This age range is consistent with previous FFSF applications in research (Mesman et al., 2009; Provenzi et al., 2016) and the manipulation of maternal responsiveness is a reasonable age-appropriate stressor. Importantly, despite the fact that FFSF procedure has been mainly used with infants less than 10 months of age (Mesman et al., 2009), limited exceptions apply to the application of this paradigm to older infants with ND (Ostfeld-Etzion et al., 2015). Moreover, it should be noted that Montirosso and Tronick (2008) and Weinberg et al. (2008) have proposed a slightly modified version of the FFSF paradigm to be used with older infants up to 30 months of age.

The FFSF procedure usually takes place in a double-room with a unidirectional mirror and with a double-cam recording system: one camera is focused on the infant and the second one on the parent. In clinical settings, adaptations of the original FFSF paradigm should be done according to different infants’ variables: age, presence of comorbidity, motor impairment, severity of the disability. For example, with very young infants (CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chow, S.-M., Haltigan, J. D., and Messinger, D. S. (2010). Dynamic infant-parent affect coupling during the face-to-face/still-face. Emotion 10, 101–114. doi: 10.1037/a0017824

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cianfaglione, R., Hastings, R. P., Felce, D., Clarke, A., and Kerr, M. P. (2015). Psychological well-being of mothers and siblings in families of girls and women with Rett syndrome. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 45, 2939–2946. doi: 10.1007/s10803-015-2457-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cohrs, A. C., and Leslie, D. L. (2017). Depression in parents of children diagnosed with autism spectrum disorder: a claims-based analysis. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 47, 1416–1422. doi: 10.1007/s10803-017-3063-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Doyle, O., Harmon, C. P., Heckman, J. J., and Tremblay, R. E. (2009). Investing in early human development: timing and economic efficiency. Econ. Hum. Biol. 7, 1–6. doi: 10.1016/j.ehb.2009.01.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dykens, E. M. (2015). Family adjustment and interventions in neurodevelopmental disorders. Curr. Opin. Psychiatry 28, 121–126. doi: 10.1097/YCO.0000000000000129

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Einfeld, S. L., Tonge, B. J., and Rees, V. W. (2001). Longitudinal course of behavioral and emotional problems in Williams syndrome. Am. J. Ment. Retard. 106, 73–81. doi: 10.1352/0895-8017(2001)1062.0.CO;2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fukkink, R. G. (2008). Video feedback in widescreen: a meta-analysis of family programs. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 28, 904–916. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2008.01.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gianino, A., and Tronick, E. Z. (1988). “The mutual regulation model: the infant’s self and interactive regulation and coping and defensive capacities,” in Stress and Coping Across Development, eds T. M. Field, P. M. McCabe, and N. Schneiderman (Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates), 47–68.

Google Scholar

Guralnick, M. J., Neville, B., Hammond, M. A., and Connor, R. T. (2008). Mothers’ social communicative adjustments to young children with mild developmental delays. Am. J. Ment. Retard. 113, 1–18. doi: 10.1352/0895-80172008113

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hauser-Cram, P., and Woodman, A. C. (2016). Trajectories of internalizing and externalizing behavior problems in children with developmental disabilities. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 44, 811–821. doi: 10.1007/s10802-015-0055-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hsu, H. C., and Jeng, S. F. (2008). Two-month-olds’ attention and affective response to maternal still face: a comparison between term and preterm infants in Taiwan. Infant Behav. Dev. 31, 194–206. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2007.10.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Innocenti, M., Roggman, L., and Cook, G. (2013). Using the PICCOLO with parents of children with disability. Infant Ment. Health J. 34, 307–318. doi: 10.1002/imhj.21394

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jahromi, L. B., Meek, S. E., and Ober-Reynolds, S. (2012). Emotion regulation in the context of frustration in children with high functioning autism and their typical peers. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 53, 1250–1258. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2012.02560.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Juffer, F., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., and Van Ijzendoorn, M. H. (eds). (2012). Promoting Positive Parenting: An Attachment-Based Intervention. Abingdon: Routledge.

Google Scholar

Kaitz, M., Maytal, H. R., Devor, N., Bergman, L., and Mankuta, D. (2010). Maternal anxiety, mother-infant interactions, and infants’ response to challenge. Infant Behav. Dev. 33, 136–148. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2009.12.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kim, J. M., and Mahoney, G. (2005). The effects of relationship focused intervention on Korean parents and their young children with disabilities. Res. Dev. Disabil. 26, 117–130. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2004.08.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lam-Cassettari, C., Wadnerkar-Kamble, M. B., and James, D. M. (2015). Enhancing parent-child communication and parental self-esteem with a video-feedback intervention: outcomes with prelingual deaf and hard-of-hearing children. J. Deaf Stud. Deaf Educ. 20, 266–274. doi: 10.1093/deafed/env008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Landry, S. H., Smith, K. E., Swank, P. R., and Guttentag, C. (2008). A responsive parenting intervention: the optimal timing across early childhood for impacting maternal behaviors and child outcomes. Dev. Psychol. 44, 1335–1353. doi: 10.1037/a0013030

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lowe, J., Handmaker, N., and Aragón, C. (2006). Impact of mother interactive style on infant affect among babies exposed to alcohol in utero. Infant Ment. Health J. 27, 371–382. doi: 10.1002/imhj.20098

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marvin, R. S., and Pianta, R. C. (1996). Mothers’ reactions to their child’s diagnosis: relations with security of attachment. J. Clin. Child Psychol. 25, 436–445. doi: 10.1207/s15374424jccp2504_8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mazefsky, C. A., and White, S. W. (2014). Emotion regulation: concepts & practice in autism spectrum disorder. Child Adolesc. Psychiatr. Clin. N. Am. 23, 15–24. doi: 10.1016/j.chc.2013.07.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mesman, J., van IJzendoorn, M. H., and Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J. (2009). The many face of the Still-Face paradigm: a review and meta-analysis. Dev. Rev. 30, 120–162. doi: 10.1016/j.dr.2009.02.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miron, D., Lewis, M. L., and Zeanah, C. H. (2009). Clinical use of observational procedures in early childhood relationship assessment. Handb. Infant Ment. Health 1, 252–265.

Google Scholar

Montirosso, R., Borgatti, R., Trojan, S., Zanini, R., and Tronick, E. (2010). A comparison of dyadic interactions and coping with still-face in healthy pre-term and full-term infants. Br. J. Dev. Psychol. 28, 347–368. doi: 10.1348/026151009X416429

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Montirosso, R., Casini, E., Provenzi, L., Putnam, S. P., Morandi, F., Fedeli, C., et al. (2015a). A categorical approach to infants’ individual differences during the Still-Face paradigm. Infant Behav. Dev. 38, 67–76. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2014.12.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Montirosso, R., Provenzi, L., Giorda, R., Fumagalli, M., Morandi, F., Sirgiovanni, I., et al. (2016a). SLC6A4 promoter region methylation and socio-emotional stress response in very preterm and full-term infants. Epigenomics 8, 895–907.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Montirosso, R., Provenzi, L., Tavian, D., Missaglia, S., Raggi, M. E., and Borgatti, R. (2016b). COMTval158met polymorphism is associated with behavioral response and physiologic reactivity to socio-emotional stress in 4-month-old infants. Infant Behav. Dev. 45, 71–82. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2016.09.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Montirosso, R., Provenzi, L., Tavian, D., Morandi, F., Bonanomi, A., Missaglia, S., et al. (2015b). Social stress regulation in 4-month-old infants: contribution of maternal social engagement and infants’ 5-HTTLPR genotype. Early Hum. Dev. 91, 173–179. doi: 10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2015.01.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Montirosso, R., Provenzi, L., Tronick, E., Morandi, F., Reni, G., and Borgatti, R. (2014). Vagal tone as a biomarker of long-term memory for a stressful social event at 4 months. Dev. Psychobiol. 56, 1564–1574. doi: 10.1002/dev.21251

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Montirosso, R., Tronick, E., Morandi, F., Ciceri, F., and Borgatti, R. (2013). Four-month-old infants’ long-term memory for a stressful social event. PLoS One 8:e82277. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0082277

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Montirosso, R., and Tronick, E. Z. (2008). “Comportamento socio-emozionale della madre e del bambino di 30 mesi e caratteristiche interattive della diade durante una versione modificata del paradigma still-face,” in Regolazione Emotiva, eds C. Riva Crugnola and C. Rodini (Milano: Raffaello Cortina).

Google Scholar

Muller, M., Zietlow, A. L., Tronick, E., and Reck, C. (2015). What dyadic reparation is meant to do: an association with infant cortisol reactivity. Psychopathology 48, 386–399. doi: 10.1159/000439225

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Odding, E., Roebroeck, M. E., and Stam, H. J. (2006). The epidemiology of cerebral palsy: incidence, impairments and risk factors. Disabil. Rehabil. 28, 183–191. doi: 10.1080/09638280500158422

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Okimoto, A. M., Bundy, A., and Hanzlik, J. (2000). Playfulness in children with and without disability: measurement and intervention. Am. J. Occup. Ther. 54, 73–82. doi: 10.5014/ajot.54.1.73

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ostfeld-Etzion, S., Golan, O., Hirschler-Guttenberg, Y., Zagoory-Sharon, O., and Feldman, R. (2015). Neuroendocrine and behavioral response to social rupture and repair in preschoolers with autism spectrum disorders interacting with mother and father. Mol. Autism 6:11. doi: 10.1186/s13229-015-0007-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Papaeliou, C., Polemikos, N., Fryssira, E., Kodakos, A., Kaila, M., Yiota, X., et al. (2012). Behavioural profile and maternal stress in Greek young children with Williams syndrome. Child Care Health Dev. 38, 844–853. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2214.2011.01306.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Papousek, M. (2007). Communication in early infancy: an arena of intersubjective learning. Infant Behav. Dev. 30, 258–266. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2007.02.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Phaneuf, L., and McIntyre, L. L. (2007). Effects of individualized video feedback combined with group parent training on inappropriate maternal behavior. J. Appl. Behav. Anal. 40, 737–741. doi: 10.1901/jaba.2007.737

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Poslawsky, I. E., Naber, F. B., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., van Daalen, E., van Engeland, H., and van IJzendoorn, M. H. (2015). Video-feedback intervention to promote positive parenting adapted to autism (VIPP-AUTI): a randomized controlled trial. Autism 19, 588–603. doi: 10.1177/1362361314537124

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Provenzi, L., Borgatti, R., Menozzi, G., and Montirosso, R. (2015a). A dynamic system analysis of dyadic flexibility and stability across the Face-to-Face Still-Face procedure: application of the state space grid. Infant Behav. Dev. 38, 1–10. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2014.10.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Provenzi, L., Casini, E., de Simone, P., Reni, G., Borgatti, R., and Montirosso, R. (2015b). Mother-infant dyadic reparation and individual differences in vagal tone affect 4-month-old infants’ social stress regulation. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 140, 158–170. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2015.07.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Provenzi, L., Fumagalli, M., Bernasconi, F., Sirgiovanni, I., Morandi, F., Borgatti, R., et al. (2017). Very preterm and full-term infants’ response to socio-emotional stress: the role of postnatal maternal bonding. Infancy 22, 695–712. doi: 10.1111/infa.12175

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Provenzi, L., Giusti, L., and Montirosso, R. (2016). Do infants exhibit significant cortisol reactivity to the Face-to-Face Still-Face paradigm? A narrative review and meta-analysis. Dev. Rev. 42, 34–55. doi: 10.1016/j.dr.2016.07.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Provenzi, L., Olson, K. L., Montirosso, R., and Tronick, E. (2016c). Infants, mothers, and dyadic contributions to stability and prediction of social stress response at 6 months. Dev. Psychol. 52, 1–8. doi: 10.1037/dev0000072

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reck, C., Müller, M., Tietz, A., and Möhler, E. (2013). Infant distress to novelty is associated with maternal anxiety disorder and especially with maternal avoidance behavior. J. Anxiety Disord. 27, 404–412. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2013.03.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sander, L. W., Amadei, G., and Bianchi, I. (2014). Living Systems, Evolving Consciousness, and the Emerging Person: A Selection of Papers from the Life Work of Louis Sander. New York, NY: Taylor and Francis Group.

Google Scholar

Schneider, B., Bouyer, S., and Mietkiewicz, M. C. (2000). Problèmes éthiques posés par quelques paradigmes de recherche (trop) classiques en psychologie et en psychopathologie développementale. Psychiatrie Enfant 43, 5–22.

Google Scholar

Segal, L. B., Oster, H., Cohen, M., Caspi, B., Myers, M., and Brown, D. (1995). Smiling and fussing in seven-month-old preterm and full-term black infants in the still-face situation. Child Dev. 66, 1829–1843. doi: 10.2307/1131913

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sigurdardottir, S., Indredavik, M. S., Eiriksdottir, A., Einarsdottir, K., Gudmundsson, H. S., and Vik, T. (2010). Behavioural and emotional symptoms of preschool children with cerebral palsy: a population-based study. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 52, 1056–1061. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8749.2010.03698.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spittle, A., Orton, J., Anderson, P. J., Boyd, R., and Doyle, L. W. (2015). Early developmental intervention programmes provided post hospital discharge to prevent motor and cognitive impairment in preterm infants. Cochrane Database Syst. Rev. 11:CD005495. doi: 10.1002/14651858.CD005495.pub4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stern, D., and Stern, D. (2012). Daniel Stern’s journey in infant psychiatry: interview by John A. Talbot. J. Nerv. Ment. Dis. 200, 1105–1108. doi: 10.1097/NMD.0b013e318275d3de

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tronick, E., Als, H., Adamson, L., Wise, S., and Brazelton, T. B. (1978). The infant’s response to entrapment between contradictory messages in face-to-face interaction. J. Am. Acad. Child Psychiatry 17, 1–13. doi: 10.1016/S0002-7138(09)62273-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tronick, E., and Beeghly, M. (2011). Infants’ meaning-making and the development of mental health problems. Am. Psychol. 66, 107–119. doi: 10.1037/a0021631

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Venuti, P., de Falco, S., Esposito, G., and Bornstein, M. H. (2009). Mother-child play: children with Down syndrome and typical development. Am. J. Intellect. Dev. Disabil. 114, 274–288. doi: 10.1352/1944-7558-114.4:274-288

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weinberg, M. K., Beeghly, M., and Olson, K. L. (2008). A still-face paradigm for young children: 2½ year-olds’ reactions to maternal unavailability during the still-face. J. Dev. Process. 3, 4–22.

Google Scholar

Weinberg, M. K., Olson, K. L., Beeghly, M., and Tronick, E. Z. (2006). Making up is hard to do, especially for mothers with high levels of depressive symptoms and their infant sons; 16790002. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry Allied Discip. 47, 670–683. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2005.01545.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weinberg, M. K., and Tronick, E. Z. (1996). Infant affective reactions to the resumption of maternal interaction after the still-face. Child Dev. 67, 905–914. doi: 10.2307/1131869

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weinberg, M. K., Tronick, E. Z., Cohn, J. F., and Olson, K. L. (1999). Gender differences in emotional expressivity and self-regulation during early infancy. Dev. Psychol. 35, 175–188. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.35.1.175

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Woolf, S. H. (2008). The meaning of translational research and why it matters. JAMA 299, 211–213. doi: 10.1001/jama.2007.26

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yato, Y., Kawai, M., Negayama, K., Sogon, S., Tomiwa, K., and Yamamoto, H. (2008). Infant responses to maternal still-face at 4 and 9 months. Infant Behav. Dev. 31, 570–577. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2008.07.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: early intervention, mother–infant interaction, neurodevelopmental disabilities, parents, still-face, rehabilitation

Citation: Giusti L, Provenzi L and Montirosso R (2018) The Face-to-Face Still-Face (FFSF) Paradigm in Clinical Settings: Socio-Emotional Regulation Assessment and Parental Support With Infants With Neurodevelopmental Disabilities. Front. Psychol. 9:789. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00789

Received: 09 February 2018; Accepted: 03 May 2018;Published: 22 May 2018.

Edited by:

Elena Nava, Università degli Studi di Milano-Bicocca, Italy

Reviewed by:

Marina Fuertes, Instituto Politécnico de Lisboa, Portugal Judi Mesman, Leiden University, Netherlands

Copyright © 2018 Giusti, Provenzi and Montirosso. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Livio Provenzi, [email protected]



【本文地址】


今日新闻


推荐新闻


CopyRight 2018-2019 办公设备维修网 版权所有 豫ICP备15022753号-3